
Oceanic squid species have a major role linking trophic levels and food webs from different habitats. Our analysis indicated that most ecosystem models inadequately include cephalopods in terms of model structure and parametrization although some models still have the capacity to draw valuable conclusions regarding the impact and role of cephalopods within the system. We examined ecosystem models from 13 regions to analyse the representation of cephalopods and compared their results to local trophic studies.


This review evaluates our representation of cephalopods in ecosystem models and the insights given by these models on the role of cephalopods in our oceans. Cephalopod ecology, however, is still poorly understood as observational studies often give highly uncertain and variable results due to the peculiarities of cephalopod behaviour and biology, and their responsiveness to external drivers. Furthermore, these results confirm that mercury bioaccumulates continuously throughout the individuals’ life, with adults doubling their mercury concentrations to juveniles.Ĭephalopods, especially squids, are believed to have a structuring role in marine ecosystems as a link between different trophic levels, primarily due to their voracious prey consumption and high production rate. Mercury values in the anterior (1.3–7.9 μg kg) subsections of the hood reflect juvenile and adult stages, respectively.

Distinct total mercury concentrations in the different subsections support that beaks can be used to study mercury levels in different periods of cephalopods’ life-cycle. tip of the rostrum and subsections along the hood). Using upper beaks of the giant warty squid Moroteuthopsis longimana (major prey in the Southern Ocean), we describe a method to assess mercury concentrations along the life of cephalopods through the segmentary analysis of beak sections (i.e. Due to the general difficulties in capturing oceanic squid, beaks found in the diet of top predators can be used to study their life-cycles and ecological role. Others say that even if octopuses are incapable of something as sophisticated as observational learning, they excel at other types of behaviours that mainstream neurobiology has long since denied in so lowly an animal.Cephalopods represent an important pathway for mercury transfer through food webs. “We have two very different brains that can do some similar things-including perhaps observational learning,” says Shelley Adamo at Dalhousie University in Halifax, Nova Scotia. After all, the brains of animals like the octopus evolved entirely separately from the brains of the vertebrates, and they have an entirely different design-perhaps they also house a unique form of intelligence. “We have many mammals that aren’t doing that.”īut not everyone is as doubtful.

“If they really did show observational learning, it would be astonishing,” she said recently. Jean Boal, who studies animal behaviour at the University of Texas in Galveston, epitomises the scepticism that greeted the announcement in 1992 of the educable octopus. But they look nothing like the brains of the vertebrates that are so adept at learning.įor that matter, why would octopuses need to learn by example? They are short-lived, solitary creatures that usually meet only once, to copulate. Octopuses, on the other hand, are molluscs, a seemingly primitive animal group. Such “observational learning” is supposed to be seen only in higher vertebrates-animals such as rats with sophisticated brains. WHEN an octopus in a research laboratory in Naples learnt to choose a red ball instead of a white one by watching another octopus, students of animal learning were taken aback.
